Abstract
Abstract
Background
Individuals on the autism spectrum commonly have differences from non-autistic people in expressing their emotions using communicative behaviors, such as facial expressions. However, it is not yet clear if this reduced expressivity stems from reduced physiological reactivity in emotional contexts or if individuals react internally, but do not show these reactions externally to others. We hypothesized that autism is characterized by a discordance between in-the-moment internal psychophysiological arousal and external communicative expressions of emotion.
Methods
Forty-one children on the autism spectrum and 39 non-autistic, typically developing (TD) children of two age groups (2–4 and 8–12 years) participated in a low-level stress task whilst wearing a wireless electrocardiogram. Children’s negative emotional expressions (facial, vocal, bodily) were coded following standardized protocols. Alexithymia traits were assessed using the Children’s Alexithymia Measure with school-aged children only. Data analyses involved ANOVAs, correlations, and sensitivity analyses.
Results
There were no group differences in physiological arousal (heart rate) or in communicative expressions of stress to the stress task. For TD preschoolers, physiological arousal during the stress task was associated with vocal expressions and for TD school-aged children, they were associated with facial and bodily expressions. By contrast, for children on the autism spectrum, physiological arousal during the stress tasks was not associated with communicative expressions across age groups.
Conclusions
Our findings suggest that children on the autism spectrum might experience emotional disconcordance, in that their physiological arousal does not align with their communicative expressions. Therefore, the internally experienced stress of children on the autism spectrum may be inadvertently missed by teachers and caregivers and, consequently, learning opportunities for teaching emotional communication and regulation may be also missed. Our results support the use of wearable biosensors to facilitate such interventions in children on the autism spectrum.
Funder
National Institute of Mental Health and Neurosciences
Publisher
Springer Science and Business Media LLC
Reference109 articles.
1. American Psychiatric Association, DSMTF, American Psychiatric Association DS. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5 (Vol. 5, No. 5). Washington, DC: American psychiatric association; 2013.
2. Bal E, Harden E, Lamb D, Van Hecke AV, Denver JW, Porges SW. Emotion recognition in children with autism spectrum disorders: Relations to eye gaze and autonomic state. J Autism Dev Disord. 2010;40(3):358–70. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-009-0884-3.
3. Barrett LF. Solving the emotion paradox: Categorization and the experience of emotion. Pers Soc Psychol Rev. 2006;10(1):20–46.
4. Barrett LF, Russell JA. An introduction to psychological construction. In: Barrett LF, Russell JA, editors. The psychological construction of emotion. New York: The Guilford Press; 2015. p. 1–17.
5. Beauchaine TP, Gatzke-Kopp L, Mead HK. Polyvagal Theory and Developmental Psychopathology: Emotion Dysregulation and Conduct Problems from Preschool to Adolescence. Biol Psychol. 2007;74(2):174–84. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.08.008.